My friend Rachel recently wrote a book review for
the Washington Post on Richard
Wrangham’s latest tome, The Goodness Paradox:
The Strange Relationship between Virtue and Violence in Human Evolution.
Rachel’s review piqued my interest, so I made it a point to get my hands on
Wrangham’s book and give it a thorough going over.
I say “going over” because I’ve long viewed
Wrangham’s theories with a serious dose of suspicion; after all, he has written
volume after volume claiming that men are violence-prone sexual predators. Obviously,
there’s no point in arguing that no men are ever like this. I mean, you’ve got
your occasional pimp, president, and Supreme Court justice that fills that
bill, but I don’t accept Wrangham’s characterization as an accurate, all-around
portrait of my gender.
I was surprised, however, to discover that some of
what Wrangham writes in The Goodness
Paradox is quite thought-provking. In particular, I thought his “execution
hypothesis” might deserve consideration. At least it stands a chance of holding
up to further scrutiny.
The very name, “execution hypothesis”, smacks of a
certain Wranghamian grisliness, but it nevertheless might make sense. At least
a little sense. Wrangham wants us to believe that we Homo sapiens have domesticated ourselves through eons of evolution
and have done so by promoting moral behavior through snuffing each other out.
His idea is not that our ancestors went about clubbing and spearing each other
willy-nilly. Rather, he believes that about a half a million years ago, human
males began focusing on the bullies in their midst and systematically executing
them. The result was a domesticated species, one in which impulsive aggression,
what he calls “reactive” aggression, was largely bred out of the bloodline.
Wrangham’s ultimate aim is to identify the
Darwinian forces that brought about human morality, hence the title of his book,
The Goodness Paradox. The quest for
the evolutionary roots of good and evil is a longstanding one, and quite a few
brilliant thinkers have speculated about it, including Darwin himself.
A common moral principle among tribal peoples, one
found in hunting and gathering societies the world over, is the rejection of
bullies and self-aggrandizing scoundrels. In hunting-gathering societies, if
you start spouting off about how you have the best words, or you know more
about hunting than the top hunters, you will quickly become an object of
mockery, derision, and social rejection. Try to bully or intimidate others into
doing your bidding and you may wind up dead.
The widespread hostility toward boastful and
bullying men, Wrangham believes, is a continuation of an ancient Pleistocene
pattern which included actual executions of the worst offenders. The consequence,
he claims, is that we have become a biologically domesticated species, one that
only rarely resorts to reactive aggression.
In our day-to-day tolerance of each other, we
resemble domestic cattle, cats and dogs more than we do non-domesticated animals
like water buffalo, lions, and wolves. Wrangham points out that domesticated
species like dogs and domestic foxes exhibit a set of physical traits which
include, for example, floppy ears, shortened faces, and white markings especially
on the face and limbs.
Non-domesticated animals are prone to reactive
aggression. Our close relative, the common chimpanzee, for example, is typically
primed to explode with reactive aggression as soon as he (we’re talking mainly
males here) senses another chimp is about to throw shade his way. Chimps,
consequently, live in noisy, rowdy troops filled with incessant bickering and
backbiting – kind of like the adults at Little League games, except the chimps
are much quicker than the human adults to spill real blood with actual backbiting. It’s the reluctance to
get viciously physical in most agonistic situations that makes us different
from undomesticated species.
Wrangham hastens to point out that humans don’t entirely
refrain from aggression, but, he argues, most human aggression is proactive,
that is, premeditated and planned rather than spontaneous. This, in fact, is
the basis of feuding, raiding, warfare, and first-degree murder, four activities
that remain widespread in the human repertoire.
I can’t say I buy Wrangham’s notion that we are a
domestic species. I mean, where are the floppy ears? Or the other markings of
domestic animals that he cites, such as white markings on face and limbs?
Still, it is true that our Pleistocene ancestors apparently
knew how to treat bullies and braggarts – they isolated them with all the
mockery and contempt they deserved. However, if this is true, it leaves us with
the question of why we don’t do the same to bullies and blowhards today. Well, it
turns out there are explanations for this, most of them having to do with
increased social complexity and the emergence of property as a weapon of social
domination. But that’s a topic for another day.
The Tribesman's Creed: "Don't tread on me, you bumptious blowhard."
I recognize the gentleman in the picture above as the sweet fellow from "The Gods Must Be Crazy". I showed that movie to my classes for many years and just thinking about it now makes me smile. One of my favorite scenes was when the young woman was having lunch in the cafeteria and turned to the person sitting next to her and asked, "Do the voices in my head bother you?". So many funny scenes. The drive to the mail box, and the unfailingly hysterical attempt to get through the gate and get it locked up again without losing the jeep!! As far as the evolution of niceness, I'll have to check out Wrangham's credentials..... it doesn't sound like he could be a biologist!
ReplyDeleteBob, I share your reaction to Wrangham's excessive stress on dominance hierarchy. I think of Christopher Boehm as being the pioneer in studying anti-dominance attitudes in neolithic cultures around the world, speculating, as Wrangham does that it arose in response to the teamwork necessary for big game hunting about 400,000 yrs ago.
ReplyDeleteI have decided that the self-domestication process happened right up front, 6 million years ago, enabling the major human adaptation all along, which has been the engagement of teamwork. I cite a mathematical modeling study that demonstrates that high cooperation was the key to the success of Homo sapiens' migrations, and the same would hold true with the migrations starting 1.8 million years ago. I think you will be surprised when you read what I have discovered in my study of the evolution of human motivations since apes started to form groups some 50 million years ago.
I am so very pleased to have made contact with such a kindred spirit, and look forward to interacting with you after you have read "Emotional Fossils,"
John